Efecto del entrenamiento de la memoria de trabajo básico en la interpretación consecutiva
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.29051/el.v7iesp.4.15603Palabras clave:
Memoria de trabajo, Capacidad de la memoria de trabajo, Interpretación consecutiva, Entrenamiento de la memoria de trabajo básicaResumen
La interpretación parece ser una de las tareas lingüísticas más exigentes y complejas, que depende en gran medida de la memoria de trabajo (MT). Este estudio intenta examinar el efecto del entrenamiento en MT en la mejora de la interpretación consecutiva. Para lograrlo, se seleccionaron 20 estudiantes de Estudios de Traducción del Instituto de Educación Superior Kashmar. La habilidad de interpretación de los participantes se calificó mediante la implementación de una escala de interpretación. Luego, todos los participantes recibieron instrucciones sobre cómo expandir su capacidad de MT a través del entrenamiento de la memoria de trabajo central. El grupo pasó por otra sesión de evaluación después del tratamiento. Los resultados se analizaron ejecutando una prueba t de muestra pareada. Se encontró que los ejercicios de expansión de la capacidad de MT tuvieron un efecto significativo en la subescala de precisión, mientras que la calidad y la entrega del idioma objetivo, las otras dos subescalas, demostraron ser menores y sin mejoras, respectivamente. Los hallazgos se pueden usar para capacitar a intérpretes o en otros entornos educativos que requieran habilidades de atención y memoria.
Descargas
Citas
ATKINSON, R. C.; SHIFFRIN, R. M. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89-195): Elsevier. 1968.
BADDELEY, A. D. Essentials of Human Memory Cognitive Psychology, 1368. 1999.
BADDELEY, A. Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of communication disorders, 36(3), 189-208. 2003.
BADDELEY, A. D.; HITCH, G. Working memory. Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89): Elsevier. 1974.
BADDELEY, A. D.; HITCH, G. J. Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485. 1994.
CONNERS, F. A.; ROSENQUIST, C. J.; ARNETT, L.; MOORE, M.; HUME, L. E. Improving memory span in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(3), 244-255. 2008.
COWAN, N. Processing limits of selective attention and working memory: Potential implications for interpreting. Interpreting, 5(2), 117-146. 2000.
COWAN, N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and brain sciences, 24(1), 87-114. 2001.
DAHLSTROM, K.; GILLBERG, C.; FORSSBERG, H.; WESTERBERG, H. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHDa randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177186. 2005.
DARÒ, V.; FABBRO, F. Verbal memory during simultaneous interpretation: Effects of phonological interference. Applied linguistics, 15(4), 365-381. 1994.
DEHN, M. J. Working memory and academic learning: Assessment and intervention: John Wiley & Sons. 2011.
DONG, Y.; LIU, Y.; CAI, R. How does consecutive interpreting training influence working memory: A longitudinal study of potential links between the two. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 875. 2018.
GATHERCOLE, S. E.; BADDELEY, A. D. Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8(3), 259-272. 1993.
HOLMES, J.; GATHERCOLE, S. E.; DUNNING, D. L. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), F9-15. 2009.
HULME, C. Language processing mechanisms and immediate memory: Possible implications for simultaneous interpreting. Ascona II Workshop on Complex cognitive processes: simultaneous interpreting as a research paradigm. 2000.
JAMES, W.; BURKHARDT, F.; BOWERS, F.; SKRUPSKELIS, I. K. The principles of psychology (Vol. 1): Macmillan London. 1890.
KÖPKE, B.; NESPOULOUS, J.-L. Working memory performance in expert and novice interpreters. Interpreting, 8(1), 1-23. 2006.
LEE, J. Rating scales for interpreting performance assessment. The interpreter and translator trainer, 2(2), 165-184. 2008.
LUSTIG, C.; SHAH, P.; SEIDLER, R.; REUTER-LORENZ, P. A. Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychology review, 19(4), 504-522. 2009.
MAHMOODZADEH, K. Consecutive interpreting: Its principles and techniques. Teaching Translation and Interpreting (pp. 231): John Benjamins. 1992.
MAKI, P. M.; MARTIN-THORMEYER, E. HIV, cognition and women. Neuropsychology review, 19(2), 204. 2009.
MEARA, P. Psycholinguistics: A Resource Book for Students: J. Field; Routledge, London, 2003, xviii+ 231 pages: Pergamon. 3004.
MILLER, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological review, 63(2), 81. 1956.
MINEAR, M.; SHAH, P. Sources of working memory deficits in children and possibilities for remediation. Working memory and education (pp. 273-307): Elsevier. 2006.
MORALES, J.; PADILLA, F.; GÓMEZ-ARIZA, C. J.; BAJO, M. T. Simultaneous interpretation selectively influences working memory and attentional networks. Acta psychologica, 155, 82-91. 2015.
MORRISON, A. B.; CHEIN, J. M. Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 18(1), 46-60. 2011.
OSAKA, M. Riidingu-supan no kenkyu 5: doujituyaku tesuto to wakingu memori (Study of reading span 5: Test of simultaneous interpreting and the working memory) Nihonshinrigakkai dai-58-kai taikai ronbunhu (Proceedings of the 58th conference of the Japan Psychological Society) (Vol. 710). 1994.
PADILLA, P. Procesos de memoria y atención en la interpretación de lenguas. PhD diss., inédita, Universidad de Granada. 1995.
PHELAN, M. The interpreter's resource: Multilingual Matters. 2001.
PRINS, P. J.; DOVIS, S.; PONSIOEN, A.; TEN BRINK, E.; VAN DER OORD, S. Does computerized working memory training with game elements enhance motivation and training efficacy in children with ADHD? Cyberpsychology, behavior, and social networking, 14(3), 115-122. 2011.
SIGNORELLI, T. M. Working memory in simultaneous interpreters: City University of New York. 2008.
SIGNORELLI, T. M.; HAARMANN, H. J.; OBLER, L. K. Working memory in simultaneous interpreters: Effects of task and age. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(2), 198-212. 2012.
TIMAROVÁ, Š.; ČEŇKOVÁ, I.; MEYLAERTS, R.; HERTOG, E.; SZMALEC, A.; DUYCK, W. Simultaneous interpreting and working memory capacity. Psycholinguistic and cognitive inquiries into translation and interpreting, 101-126. 2015.
TURLEY-AMES, K. J.; WHITFIELD, M. M. Strategy training and working memory task performance. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 446-468. 2003.
Publicado
Cómo citar
Número
Sección
Licencia
Esta obra está bajo una licencia internacional Creative Commons Atribución-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0.
Os manuscritos aceitos e publicados são de propriedade da Revista EntreLínguas. Os artigos publicados e as referências citadas na Revista EntreLínguas são de inteira responsabilidade de seus autores.
Transferência de direitos autorais – autorização para publicação
Caso o artigo submetido seja aprovado para publicação, já fica acordado que o(s) autor(es) autoriza(m) a UNESP a reproduzi-lo e publicá-lo na EntreLínguas, entendendo-se os termos “reprodução” e “publicação” conforme definição respectivamente dos incisos VI e I do artigo 5° da Lei 9610/98. O artigo poderá ser acessado pela rede mundial de computadores (Internet), sendo permitidas, a título gratuito, a consulta e a reprodução de exemplar do artigo para uso próprio de quem a consulta, desde que haja a citação ao texto consultado. Essa autorização de publicação 328 EntreLínguas, Araraquara, v. 1, n .2, p. 323-328, jul./dez. 2015 não tem limitação de tempo, ficando a UNESP responsável pela manutenção da identificação do(s) autor(es) do artigo. Os artigos publicados e as referências citadas na Revista EntreLínguas são de inteira responsabilidade de seus autores.